JOURNAL
OF
APPLIED
BEHAVIOR
ANALYSIS
TEACHING
CHILDREN
WITH
AUTISM
TO
USE
PHOTOGRAPHIC
ACTIVITY
SCHEDULES:
MAINTENANCE
AND
GENERALIZATION
OF
COMPLEX
RESPONSE
CHAINS
GREGORY
S.
MACDUFF,
PATRICIA
J.
KRANTZ,
AND
LYNN
E.
MCCLANNAHAN
PRINCETON
CHILD
DEVELOPMENT
INSTITUTE
We
used
a
graduated
guidance
procedure
to
teach
4
boys
with
autism
to
follow
photographic
activity
schedules
to
increase
on-task
and
on-schedule
behavior.
The
multiple
baseline
across
par-
ticipants
design
induded
baseline,
teaching,
maintenance,
resequencing
of
photographs,
and
gen-
eralization
to
novel
photographs
phases.
The
results
indicated
that
photographic
activity
schedules
(albums
depicting
after-school
activities)
produced
sustained
engagement,
and
skills
generalized
to
a
new
sequence
of
photographs
and
to
new
photographs.
The
acquisition
of
schedule-following
skills
enabled
these
children
with
severe
developmental
disabilities
to
display
lengthy
response
chains,
independently
change
activities,
and
change
activities
in
different
group
home
settings
in
the
absence
of
immediate
supervision
and
prompts
from
others.
DESCRIPTORS:
autism,
prompting,
stimulus
control,
photographic
cues,
generalization
A
goal
of
behavioral
intervention
for
people
with
autism
is
the
development
of
functional
skills
that
maximize
engagement
in
appropriate
self-care,
work,
and
leisure
activities.
However,
many
inter-
vention
packages
rely
heavily
on
verbal
instructions,
modeling,
and
gestures.
Because
these
prompts
are
often
associated
with
reinforcement
during
teach-
ing,
they
may
acquire
stimulus
control
over
target
responses,
with
the
result
that
learners
may
not
display
target
skills
in
the
absence
of
teachers
and
prompting
procedures.
Although
complex
behav-
ioral
repertoires
may
be
quickly
established
in
a
treatment
setting,
often
they
do
not
generalize
or
endure
over
time
(see
Dunlap
&
Plienis,
1988;
Fowler,
1988;
Sailor,
Goetz,
Anderson,
Hunt,
&
Gee,
1988).
Children
with
autism
who
have
been
taught
to
play
with
toys,
dust
the
furniture,
and
complete
handwriting
worksheets
or
other
func-
tional
skills
often
fail
to
exhibit
these
responses
spontaneously,
perhaps
because
stimulus
control
did
not
transfer
from
prompts
embedded
in
the
training
setting
to
naturally
occurring
stimuli
(Bil-
lingsley
&
Romer,
1983;
Snell,
1983).
Reprints
may
be
obtained
from
the
authors,
Princeton
Child
Development
Institute,
300
Cold
Soil
Road,
Princeton,
New
Jersey
08540.
People
with
autism
have
also
had
difficulty
ac-
quiring
lengthy
response
chains.
Thus,
although
a
boy
may
have
learned
to
brush
his
teeth,
get
dressed,
and
go
to
breakfast,
he
may
not
complete
this
sequence
without
prompts
to
initiate
each
activity
or
each
part
of
an
activity.
Some
investigators
have
used
visual
cues
(pictures,
photographs,
or
line
drawings)
as
sequenced
discriminative
stimuli
that
enable
participants
to
complete
the
steps
in
a
com-
plex
task
or
to
change
tasks
independently.
For
example,
Wacker
and
Berg
(1983)
used
photo-
graphs
of
line
drawings
to
teach
adolescents
with
moderate
and
severe
retardation
to
complete
com-
plex
assembly
tasks
consisting
of
18
to
30
steps,
andJohnson
and
Cuvo
(1981)
used
pictorial
recipes
to
help
adults
with
developmental
disabilities
learn
cooking
skills
such
as
boiling,
baking,
and
broiling.
Others
have
used
visual
cues
to
help
people
with
developmental
disabilities
acquire
self-care
skills
(Thinesen
&
Bryan,
1981),
meal
preparation
and
cooking
repertoires
(Martin,
Rusch,
James,
Decker,
&
Trtol,
1982;
Robinson-Wilson,
1977),
com-
puter
use
(Frank,
Wacker,
Berg,
&
McMahon,
1985),
and
derical
and
laundry
tasks
(Wacker,
Berg,
Berrie,
&
Swatta,
1985).
Sowers,
Rusch,
Connis,
and
Cummings
(1980)
taught
adults
with
mental
retardation
to
move
independently
from
89
1993,
269,
89-97
NUMBEEL
1
(SPRING
1993)
GREGORY
S.
MAcDUFF
et
al.
work
to
lunch
or
break
and
back
to
work;
their
research
is
noteworthy
because
there
have
been
few
examinations
of
the
use
of
picture
cues
to
teach
people
to
move
independently
from
an
activity
in
one
setting
to
a
different
activity
in
another
setting.
Although
some
investigations
have
shown
that
the
use
of
visual
cues
can
decrease
dependence
on
teachers
and
enhance
generalization
and
mainte-
nance
of
newly
acquired
skills,
most
of
these
studies
have
used
intervention
packages
that
combine
pic-
tures,
line
drawings,
or
photographs
with
other
procedures.
Research
using
visual
cueing
procedures
has
not
yet
clarified
which
components
of
treatment
packages
are
responsible
for
the
reported
results,
or
whether
entire
treatment
packages
are
essential.
Our
investigation
was
designed
to
assess
the
effects
of
a
two-component
teaching
package
(photo-
graphs
and
graduated
guidance)
on
participants'
on-task
and
on-schedule
behavior.
Our
goal
is
to
extend
the
literature
with
4
youths
whose
group-
home
intervention
programs
relied
on
verbal
con-
tacts
(instructions,
questions,
and
praise
statements)
by
caregivers.
Each
youth
received
an
average
of
one
or
two
verbal
contacts
per
minute.
Although
this
rate
of
staff
contacts
resulted
in
very
high
levels
of
appropriate
engagement
and
low
levels
of
ste-
reotypic
behavior,
most
responses
were
prompted,
and
previously
taught
skills
were
seldom
sponta-
neously
displayed.
Several
attempts
to fade
verbal
prompts
had
been
abandoned
because
decreases
in
prompts
were
associated
with
increases
in
off-task
and
disruptive
behavior.
In
this
context,
we
ex-
amined
the
effects
of
photographic
activity
sched-
ules,
taught
with
graduated
guidance,
on
the
ac-
quisition,
maintenance,
and
generalization
of
complex
response
chains
that
required
the
partici-
pants
to
remain
engaged
and
to
move
into
different
settings
in
their
group
home
without
prompts.
METHOD
Participants
The
4
participants
in
this
study
were
Mike
and
Walter,
age
9;
Steve,
age
11;
and
Roy,
age
14.
They
met
the
DSM-III-R
(APA,
1987)
criteria
for
autism,
and
an
independent
diagnosis
of
autism
had
been
conferred
by
outside
agencies
before
their
program
enrollment.
The
boys'
scores
on
the
Pea-
body
Picture
Vocabulary
Test
ranged
from
2.1
to
3.9
years
(M
=
3.2);
Steve
was
unable
to
obtain
a
basal
score.
Age-equivalent
scores
on
the
Vineland
Adaptive
Behavior
Scale
were
5.5
for
Mike
and
Walter,
5.3
for
Roy,
and
3.3
for
Steve.
Informed
parental
consent
was
obtained
for
each
participant.
All
participants
had
long
histories
of
disruptive
behavior,
including
aggression,
tantrums,
and
run-
ning
away,
and
all
displayed
high
rates
of
stereo-
typic
behavior
in
the
absence
of
structured
pro-
gramming.
In
addition,
all
had
severe
language
deficits;
they
exhibited
echolalia,
vocal
noise,
non-
contextual
speech,
and
lack
of
spontaneous
lan-
guage.
The
boys
were
dependent
on
ongoing
su-
pervision
and
verbal
prompts
to
complete
self-help,
housekeeping,
and
leisure
activities.
Prior
to
this
investigation,
incidental
teaching
and
discrete-trial
teaching
procedures
were
used
to
teach
home-living
skills
such
as
vacuuming,
dusting,
and
table
setting,
as
well
as
leisure
skills
such
as
using
manipulative
toys,
biking,
and
rollerskating.
All
participants
had
acquired
picture-object
correspondence
skills
before
the
study
began,
and
each
had
a
limited
experience
with
a
photographic
activity
schedule
that
either
(a)
displayed
the
steps
in
preparing
a
bag
lunch
for
the
next
school
day
or
(b)
depicted
the
steps
necessary
to
obtain
a
preferred
beverage.
None
had
ever
used
photographic
cues
that
prompted
leisure
skills
or
that
depicted
a
sequence
of
different
ac-
tivities.
Setting
The
study
was
conducted
in
a
community-based
Teaching-Family
Model
group
home
where
the
participants
had
resided
for
1.1
to
4.2
years
(M
=
2.1
years).
The
home
was
staffed
by
live-in
teaching
parents
(a
married
couple)
and
two
other
full-time
therapists.
Like
other
Teaching-Family
programs,
the
home
was
family-style
and
consumer-evaluated
(McClannahan,
Krantz,
McGee,
&
MacDuff,
1984).
Sessions
were
conducted
in
the
living
room,
family
room,
and
participants'
bedrooms.
Photographic
Activity
Schedules
Each
participant's
photographic
activity
schedule
was
displayed
in
a
three-ring
binder.
Each
binder
90
PHOTOGRAPHIC
ACTIVITY
SCHEDULES
contained
six
photographs
(35
mm)
depicting
lei-
sure
and
homework
activities;
each
photo
(7
cm
by
1
1.5
cm)
was
mounted
in
the
center
of
a
single
page
of
white
paper
(21.5
cm
by
28
cm)
that
was
inserted
in
a
plastic
page
protector.
Photographs
displayed
materials
against
a
plain
background
without
distractors;
for
example,
a
picture
of
a
snack
showed
only
a
plate
with
two
cookies.
Initially,
the
first
three
photographs
in
Mike's
schedule
were
Colorforms
®,
handwriting
work-
sheets,
and
Tinker
Toys®0;
Walt's
first,
second,
and
third
photos
were
handwriting
worksheets,
Lincoln
Logs®,
and
Colorforms®0;
Roy's
were
Memory®
game,
Lego®
blocks,
and
Cootie®
game;
and
Steve's
were
Lego
blocks,
Colorforms
®,
and
Per-
fection®
.
The
three
remaining
activities
in
the
boys'
schedules
were
snack,
puzzle,
and
TV
in
the
fourth,
fifth,
and
sixth
positions,
respectively.
Some
of
the
leisure
materials
depicted
in
the
photographic
ac-
tivity
schedules
were
displayed
on
shelves
above
the
youths'
desks,
some
were
located
on
their
dress-
ers,
and
others
(e.g.,
the
TV)
were
located
several
rooms
away
from
the
boys'
bedrooms
in
the
family
room.
Dependent
Variables
On-task.
On-task
was
recorded
if
participants
were
(a)
visually
attending
to
any
appropriate
play
or
work
materials,
(b)
looking
at
their
photographic
schedules,
(c)
manipulating
play
or
work
materials
appropriately
(i.e.,
as
they
were
designed
to
be
used),
or
(d)
in
transition
from
one
scheduled
ac-
tivity
to
another.
Off-task
was
scored
if
they
(a)
used
materials
in
a
manner
other
than
that
for
which
they
were
designed,
(b)
manipulated
but
did
not
visually
attend
to
the
materials,
(c)
engaged
in
inappropriate
behavior
(e.g.,
aggression,
tantrums,
stereotypies),
or
(d)
did
not
engage
in
activities
or
use
materials.
On-schedule.
On-schedule
was
scored
if,
at
the
moment
of
observation,
a
participant
was
engaged
in
the
activity
depicted
on
the
page
to
which
his
activity
schedule
was
open.
For
example,
if
a
boy
was
building
with
Lego®
blocks
and
his
notebook
was
open
to
a
page
displaying
a
photograph
of
Lego
blocks,
on-schedule
was
scored.
However,
if
he
was
using
the
blocks
and
the
photographic
activity
schedule
showed
handwriting
worksheets,
the
observers
scored
on-task
but
off-schedule.
Fur-
ther,
off-schedule
was
recorded
if
on-task
criteria
were
not
met.
Independent
Variables
Verbal
contacts.
These
were
defined
as
verbal
instructions,
questions,
or
praise
statements
(e.g.,
"Steve,
look
at
your
puzzle,"
"What
color
will
you
use,
Roy?"
or
"Good,
Mike,
you're
looking
at
your
blocks!").
Gestures
and
gestural
prompts.
These
prompts
induded
all
pointing,
motioning,
or
nodding
to-
ward
children
or
materials,
as
well
as
pointing
to
specific
toys,
materials,
or
photographs
that
rep-
resented
the
next
tasks
in
a
sequence.
Thus,
both
nonspecific
gestures
(e.g.,
pointing
toward
a
child)
and
gestural
prompts
(e.g.,
pointing
to
the
last
piece
to
be
placed
in
a
puzzle)
were
scored.
This
broad
definition
was
used
to
identify
any
trainer
behavior
that
could
potentially
influence
partici-
pants'
performance.
Manualprompts.
Manual
prompts
were
defined
as
orienting
a
youth's
head
toward
materials,
hand-
over-hand
prompts,
and
light
touches
such
as
those
that
occur
when
manual
guidance
is
faded.
Measurement
Procedures
During
all
sessions,
independent
observers
used
a
60-s
momentary
time-sampling
procedure
to
score
on-task
and
on-schedule.
Additional
observers
re-
corded
verbal
contacts,
gestures
and
gestural
prompts,
and
manual
prompts
with
a
60-s
partial-
interval
procedure.
Experimental
Design
A
multiple
baseline
design
across
participants
was
used
to
assess
the
effects
of
photographic
ac-
tivity
schedules
on
on-task
and
on-schedule
be-
havior
during
baseline,
teaching,
maintenance,
re-
sequencing
of
pictorial
schedules,
and
generalization
to
novel
photographs.
Experimental
Conditions
Sessions
were
60
min
in
duration.
Prior
to
all
sessions,
participants
were
seated
on
a
bench
in
the
living
room;
their
photographic
activity
schedules
GREGORY
S.
MAcDUFF
et
al.
were
located
on
a
table
approximately
1
m
from
the
bench
and
directly
in
front
of
them.
Activity
schedules
and
the
depicted
materials
were
present
during
all
conditions.
Sessions
began
when
the
pri-
mary
data
collector
gave
the
instruction,
"Everyone
look
at
me;
please
find
something
to
do."
This
standard
instruction
was
used
throughout
all
phases
of
the
research,
and
no
rewards
were
delivered
by
the
teacher.
Baseline.
After
the
standard
instruction
was
giv-
en,
no
additional
manual,
gestural,
or
verbal
prompts
were
delivered,
and
inappropriate
behavior
was
ignored.
The
teacher
(the
first
author)
was
never
present
during
baseline.
Teaching
use
of
pictorial
schedules.
Partici-
pants
successively
entered
the
teaching
condition.
At
first,
the
teacher
stood
next
to
the
bench
where
the
boys
were
seated,
and
after
the
primary
observer
gave
the
initial
instruction,
the
teacher
waited
10
s
for
a
target
child
to
stand
up.
If
the
participant
did
not
get
up
during
this
interval,
the
teacher
placed
his
hand
on
the
boy's
shoulder
and
manually
guided
him
to
his
photographic
activity
schedule.
If
the
child
got
up
but
did
not
move
toward
the
photographic
schedule,
the
same
prompt
was
de-
livered.
Graduated
guidance,
delivered
from
be-
hind
the
youth,
was
used
to
help
him
complete
the
sequence
of
activities
pictured
in
his
activity
sched-
ule,
in
the
order
in
which
photographs
were
pre-
sented.
During
initial
sessions,
the
boy
was
man-
ually
prompted
to
pick
up
his
notebook,
carry
it
to
his
bedroom,
open
it,
point
to
the
first
picture,
gather
the
necessary
materials,
complete
the
activ-
ity,
put
materials
away,
and
turn
the
page
to
the
next
activity.
A
child
was
manually
prompted
to
put
materials
away
and
move
on
to
the
next
activity
when
he
(a)
used
all
the
available
materials
(e.g.,
if
30
Lego®
blocks
were
provided,
he
used
all
pieces)
or
(b)
completed
all
items
on
his
worksheets.
If
a
participant
progressed
to
the
last
scheduled
activity
(TV)
before
60
min
had
elapsed,
he
con-
tinued
this
activity
until
the
session
ended.
The
teaching
procedure
specifically
prohibited
verbal
contacts,
gestures,
and
gestural
prompts,
and
called
for
the
preventive
use
of
manual
prompts
to
de-
crease
errors.
Manual
prompts
were
always
delivered
from
be-
hind
the
youth.
Graduated
guidance
was
initially
available
for
all
tasks
specified
by
the
photographic
schedule,
but
prompts
were
faded
in
frequency
and
intensity
as
rapidly
as
possible.
Fading
began
by
moving
from
graduated
guidance
to
spatial
fading
(i.e.,
the
teacher
changed
the
location
of
manual
prompts).
Subsequently,
the
teacher
moved
to
shadowing-he
followed
the
youth's
movements
with
his
hands
near
the
boy,
but
without
making
physical
contact
(Cooper,
1987;
Foxx
&
Azrin,
1973).
However,
if
a
boy
engaged
in
inappropriate
behavior,
or
if
he
paused
for
an
extended
period
of
time,
prompts
were
reinstated.
When
a
youth
was
scored
as
on-task
and
on-
schedule
during
at
least
80%
of
time
samples
with
shadowing,
the
teacher
began
to
fade
his
physical
proximity.
The
teaching
condition
ended
for
a
youth
when
he
remained
on-task
and
on-schedule
during
at
least
80%
of
time
samples
for
five
consecutive
sessions
after
the
teacher's
physical
proximity
had
been
faded.
(Because
Mike
and
Walter
shared
one
bedroom
and
Roy
and
Steve
shared
another,
the
teacher
was
present
in
each
bedroom
until
his
prox-
imity
was
faded
for
the
second
boy
in
each
dyad.
The
teacher
was
present
in
the
living
room
and
family
room
until
his
proximity
was
faded
for
Steve.)
Maintenance.
During
maintenance,
the
teacher
was
present
to
prompt
the
youth
entering
the
teach-
ing
condition,
but
boys
in
maintenance
received
no
prompts.
For
example,
although
the
teacher
stood
next
to
the
bench
at
the
beginning
of
the
session
or
was
present
in
the
room
of
a
child
who
had
recently
completed
training,
he
did
not
provide
any
prompts
to
participants
who
had
completed
train-
ing.
Resequencing
photographic
activity
schedules.
At
the
beginning
of
this
condition,
four
of
the
six
original
activities
in
Mike's,
Walter's,
and
Roy's
schedules
were
resequenced
(i.e.,
all
activities
except
snack
and
TV
were
randomly
assigned
to
new
po-
sitions
in
their
schedules).
All
other
aspects
of
the
schedules
remained
constant,
no
prompts
were
de-
livered,
and
the
teacher
was
absent.
This
manip-
ulation
was
performed
to
assess
whether
the
boys
were
using
their
schedules
or
following
now-fa-
92
PHOTOGRAPHIC
ACTIVITY
SCHEDULES
miliar
routines.
Because
of
time
constraints,
Steve
did
not
participate
in
the
resequencing
condition.
Generalization.
The
teacher
was
absent
throughout
this
condition.
During
the
generaliza-
tion
phase,
two
of
the
six
original
activities
were
replaced
with
two
similar
but
novel
leisure
activities
for
each
boy.
In
Mike's
schedule,
Colorforms
®
and
Tinker
Toys®
were
replaced
by
a
marble
game
and
coloring;
in
Walt's,
Lincoln
Logs®0
and
Color-
forms®
were
replaced
by
Pipeworks®0
and
Lego®0
blocks;
in
Roy's,
Memory®
and
Cootie®
games
were
replaced
by
Perfection
®
and
a
peg
game;
and
in
Steve's
schedule,
Legions
of
Power®0
and
Ra-
magon®
replaced
Lego®
blocks
and
Perfection
®.
Although
these
new
activities
called
on
previously
acquired
skills,
none
had
ever
been
the
topic
of
instruction,
nor
had
any
of
them
been
components
of
photographic
activity
schedules.
Interobserver
Agreement
Interobserver
agreement
data
were
obtained
for
the
dependent
and
the
independent
variables
in
at
least
30%
of
sessions
across
all
conditions.
Interval-
by-interval
percentage
interobserver
agreement
was
calculated
by
dividing
the
number
of
agreements
by
the
number
of
agreements
plus
disagreements
and
multiplying
by
100%.
Mean
interobserver
agreement
on
the
occurrence
of
on-task
was
96%
(range,
0%
to
100%),
and
mean
agreement
on
nonoccurrence
of
on-task
was
95%
(range,
0%
to
100%).
For
on-schedule,
mean
agreement
was
99%
for
both
occurrence
and
nonoccurrence,
and
ranges
for
both
were
98%
to
100%.
No
verbal
or
gestural
prompts
were
ever
scored;
interobserver
agreement
on
nonoccurrence
was
100%.
Mean
agreement
on
the
occurrence
of
manual
prompts
was
99%
(range,
50%
to
100%),
and
mean
agreement
on
nonoc-
currence
of
manual
prompts
was
99%
(range,
99%
to
100%).
RESULTS
On-Task
Figure
1
shows
levels
of
on-task
for
the
4
par-
ticipants
across
all
conditions.
During
baseline,
the
boys,
with
the
exception
of
Steve,
displayed
con-
siderable
variability
across
sessions;
on-task
was
al-
most
never
scored
for
Steve.
With
each
application
of
teaching
pictorial
schedules,
on-task
immediately
increased
for
all
youths.
Mean
on-task
performance
was
99%
for
Mike,
Walt,
and
Roy,
and
97%
for
Steve.
During
maintenance,
resequencing
of
photographs,
and
generalization
to
novel
photographs,
all
youths
dis-
played
high
and
stable
on-task
performances.
Means
for
Mike
and
Walt
across
these
three
conditions
were
99%;
Roy's
on-task
means
were
97%
in
main-
tenance,
96%
in
resequencing,
and
97%
in
gen-
eralization.
Finally,
Steve's
on-task
averaged
91%
in
maintenance
and
96%
during
generalization;
he
did
not
participate
in
the
picture-resequencing
phase.
On-Schedule
Figure
2
reports
the
participants'
on-schedule
data
during
all
experimental
conditions.
During
baseline,
on-schedule
was
never
scored
for
any
of
the
youths.
Teaching
pictorial
schedules
produced
on-schedule
means
of
99%
for
Mike,
Walter,
and
Roy,
and
96%
for
Steve.
In
maintenance,
on-sched-
ule
means
were
98%, 99%,
97%,
and
91%
for
Mike,
Walter,
Roy,
and
Steve,
respectively.
Fur-
ther,
the
boys
continued
to
use
their
schedules
after
the
photographs
were
resequenced,
obtaining
mean
on-schedule
scores
of
97%
(Mike),
99%
(Walter),
and
95%
(Roy).
The
presentation
of
novel
pho-
tographs
and
materials
during
the
generalization
phase
produced
on-schedule
means
of
99%
for
Mike
and
Walter,
97%
for
Roy,
and
96%
for
Steve.
Independent
Variables
During
baseline,
no
verbal
contacts,
gestures
or
gestural
prompts,
or
manual
prompts
were
record-
ed.
In
the
first
five
sessions
of
the
teaching
phase,
the
percentage
of
intervals
scored
for
the
occurrence
of
manual
prompts
ranged
from
3%
to
8%
for
Mike
(M
=
4%),
0%
to
22%
for
Walt
(M
=
8%),
20%
to
40%
for
Roy
(M
=
23%),
and
8%
to
37%
(M
=
19%)
for
Steve.
The
last
five
training
sessions
for
each
participant
contained
no
manual
prompts,
and
no
verbal
contacts,
gestures,
or
ges-
tural
prompts
were
scored
throughout
this
condi-
tion.
No
prompts
of
any
type
were
scored
during
GREGORY
S.
MAcDUFF
et
al.
Un
a)
u
-,a)
cn
(v
4-
r
CU
a
u_
co
4)
2-
e
I
..
... ...
I
....
I-
IM
W
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
a)
C:
C:
4C
r-
C
U)
U
=
2)
V
)
A&
-
-0-146-40"
%.a-
-
-
%
On-Task
Manual
Prompts
84
36
-_
-
_ _ _ _
_-_-
96
148
60
72
84
i
96
10
I-
- - - - -
-
-
--I
I
I
~~~~~~I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I_
_ _ _ _ _
I
I
l
X
l
l
l~~~~~~~~
I
Il[
*
j
j
:
-
it
j
j
z
s
^:
I
I
~~~~I
148
60
72
84
1
96
1
10
I
I
I
_ _ _
_ _ _ _
__
__ _ _ _
III
I
I
I~~~~~~~~~~~~
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I.I
IA-..
A....
0
12
24
4
36
48
60
72
84
96
SESSIONS
Figure
1.
Percentage
of
60-s
momentary
time
samples
scored
as
on-task,
and
percentage
of
60-s
intervals
scored
for
manual
prompts
during
1-hr
sessions.
94
c
4Ji
N)
C-
a)
C
80
60
40
20
80
60
40
20
108
<H
I
-
zo
0
oc
(1)0.
<Ix
<
cr
0
LL
U.
c
0
o
LLw
uc
(1)0
u
LI
0L-J
LLQ
F
Hz
Ucr
0
L
'8
~~~~~~~~~~~I
I
I
100-
80-
60-
40
-
20
)8
108
r-
I
o
I-
I
I
.
1L.....
..
....
-Ir
-
-
111
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
it
-5
I
I
I
I
r-
I
I
I
I
I
I
-I
Ar
-
iazr
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
-A
as---D&A-
PHOTOGRAPHIC
ACTIVITY
SCHEDULES
C_
_
_E
L.
a)
U4)
_
,
en
a)-
I-_
cn
a)
a)
U-)
CY)
0
4)~
4)
C
_
r
0
m~~~
4-,D
,
aL)
_-n
r
c
m
4c
-
u
W
T-
c
c-
(.n
C.D
0
1
2
241
36
48 60
72
84
1
96
g
108
L
I
O
-
1
A~10".jl
I
..JaL".
A
D
,
,
,
p~~~~~~~~~~~ALTER
O-
_ _ _ _ _ _ _
__._
_ _ _
_._
_
0
1
2
241
3
6
1
48
----6
0
72
84
96
108
O-6
AAA
!,
I
I
I
I
,I
0
0-
o
~~~~I
I
Im
*
0
0
1
2
24
36
48
60
72
84
96
108
SESSIONS
Figure
2.
Percentage
of
60-s
time
samples
scored
as
on-schedule
during
1-hr
sessions.
95
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
lOC
80
60
40
20
LLJ
I
Z
8C
0
(O~
6C
Ar
IL
z
2(
0
,
4)
(n
LL
Q
104
<
8(
(I-)
iili
64
LL
21
0
Lii
In
I
z
LiU
10
W
8C
0~
6'
44
21
n-
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
AAAAAAAAAAAA.A
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
hAdw
AAM
"A
11
664
ri
-di
-ai
a
4
A
4
bm"
A
4
AA
04"
p
Mfi&AA AA
r
-
-
V
-
la
A-
i
to
-
l
I
I
I
I
I
GREGORY
S.
MAcDUFF
et
al.
maintenance,
resequencing,
or
generalization
phases.
The
boys
completed
these
sessions
without
assis-
tance
or
guidance,
and
from
Session
90
on,
the
teacher
was
no
longer
present.
DISCUSSION
Clinical
data
indicated
that
the
4
participants
had
learned
to
do
a
variety
of
recreational
and
home-living
tasks,
but
baseline
data
showed
that
the
presence
of
leisure
and
homework
materials
did
not
prompt
sustained
engagement.
During
the
teaching
condition,
however,
participants
met
cri-
terion
in
only
13
to
27
sessions;
subsequently,
they
continued
to
be
on-task
and
on-schedule
without
prompts
from
the
teacher,
and
their
schedule-fol-
lowing
skills
generalized
to
new
sequences
of
ac-
tivities
and
to
novel
photographs
and
materials
with
no
additional
training.
The
photographic
schedules
enabled
the
boys
to
display
lengthy
and
complex
chains
of
previously
mastered,
functional
behavior.
Anecdotally,
it
also
appeared
that
when
following
their
schedules,
they
engaged
in
fewer
aberrant
be-
haviors;
this
may
be
a
topic
for
future
research.
The
present
study
extends
previous
investiga-
tions
of
pictorial
and
photographic
cues
in
several
ways.
First,
prompts
were
specified
and
measured.
Most
earlier
studies
used
training
packages
that
included
multiple
prompting
procedures,
making
an
analysis
of
the
effects
of
specific
classes
of
training
stimuli
difficult
or
impossible.
Only
one
other
study
of
visual
cues
measured
all
of
the
prompts
used
in
training
(Johnson
&
Cuvo,
1981);
our
research
isolated
the
effects
of
graduated
guidance.
Our
in-
vestigation
also
measured
another
variable,
on-
schedule,
that
has
been
frequently
discussed
but
seldom
assessed.
Several
other
researchers
have
re-
ported
that
participants
eventually
discontinued
or
modified
their
use
of
photographs
or
pictures.
For
example,
Thinesen
and
Bryan
(1981)
reported
that
after
1
week
of
training,
all
15
participants
stopped
paging
through
their
albums,
using
only
the
first
or
a
randomly
selected
photograph
to
complete
an
entire
grooming
sequence.
Connis
(1979)
noted
that
subjects
eventually
stopped
checking
each
pic-
ture
before
changing
work
tasks.
In
our
study,
the
resequencing
and
generalization
conditions
estab-
lished
that
the
photographs
were
relevant
discrim-
inative
stimuli,
and
that
participants
were
not
mere-
ly
engaging
in
familiar
routines.
The
participants'
consistent
use
of
schedules
may
be
related
to
the
training
procedures.
The
trainer
ensured
that
the
boys
turned
single
pages,
pointed
to
photographs
before
beginning
tasks,
obtained
the
depicted
materials,
and
completed
scheduled
activities
in
sequence.
Graduated
guidance
was
used
to
prevent
errors,
lengthy
delays
that
might
have
interrupted
response
chains,
and
stereotypies
that
might
otherwise
have
become
embedded
in
re-
sponse
chains.
The
boys
continued
to
demonstrate
look-then-do
sequences
throughout
the
study.
The
nature
of
the
target
tasks
may
also
influence
the
posttraining
use
of
visual
cues.
People
who
engage
in
one
repetitive
task
(e.g.,
assembly
or
packaging
tasks)
may
cease
to
use
visual
prompts
because
tasks
are
familiar
and
unchanging.
But
activities
that
are
frequently
resequenced
(e.g.,
gro-
cery
shopping
or
work
assignments
in
an
industrial
kitchen)
may
require
continued
reference
to
pic-
torial
cues.
It
is
interesting
that
in
the
current
investigation,
boys
not
yet
in
training
were
never
observed
to
imitate
the
schedule-following
behavior
of
those
in
the
teaching
phase.
Further,
no
resistance
to
manual
guidance
was
noted.
After
training,
off-task
was
occasionally
scored,
but
children
were
not
observed
to
engage
in
functional
activities
other
than
those
cued
by
their
photographic
schedules.
The
relatively
small
number
of
manual
prompts
required
to
teach
the
participants
to
follow
schedules
may
have
been
related
to
(a)
their
prior
mastery
of
picture-object
correspondence
skills,
(b)
their
previous
acquisition
of
the
types
of
skills
needed
to
complete
depicted
tasks,
and
(c)
the
fading
of
graduated
guidance.
The
manual
prompting
procedure
was
selected
be-
cause
it
was
hoped
that
nonverbal
prompts
deliv-
ered
from
behind
a
youth
would
prevent
perfor-
mance
from
becoming
dependent
on
the
teacher's
presence.
Although
least-to-most
prompt
sequences
are
frequently
used
with
participants
such
as
these,
the
most-to-least
sequence
described
above
was
ef-
fective
in
preventing
errors
that
might
otherwise
96
PHOTOGRAPHIC
ACTIVITY
SCHEDULES
97
have
become
embedded
in
schedule-following
re-
sponse
chains.
Before
this
investigation
began,
the
4
partici-
pants
were
dependent
upon
verbal
prompts
to
re-
main
constructively
engaged
in
appropriate
activ-
ities.
In
the
absence
of
verbal
prompts
from
supervising
adults,
it
appeared
that
stimulus
control
transferred
to
photographs
and
materials
that
were
available
in
the
group
home.
When
the
study
end-
ed,
all
4
boys
were
able
to
display
complex
home-
living
and
recreational
repertoires
for
an
hour,
dur-
ing
which
time
they
frequently
changed
tasks
and
moved
to
different
areas
of
their
group
home
with-
out
adults'
prompts.
Photographic
activity
sched-
ules,
taught
with
graduated
guidance,
became
func-
tional
discriminative
stimuli
that
promoted
sustained
engagement
after
training
ceased
and
fostered
gen-
eralized
responding
to
new
activity
sequences
and
novel
leisure
materials.
REFERENCES
American
Psychiatric
Association.
(1987).
Diagnostic
and
statistical
manual
of
mental
disorders
(3rd
ed.
rev.).
Washington,
DC:
Author.
Billingsley,
F.
F.,
&
Romer,
L.
T.
(1983).
Response
prompting
and
the
transfer
of
stimulus
control:
Methods,
research,
and
a
conceptual
framework.
Journal
of
the
Association
for
the
Severely
Handicapped,
8,
3-12.
Connis,
R.
T.
(1979).
The
effects
of
sequential
pictorial
cues,
self-recording,
and
praise
on
the
job
task
sequencing
of
retarded
adults.
Journal
of
Applied
Behavior
Anal-
ysis,
12,
353-362.
Cooper,
J.
0.
(1987).
Stimulus
control.
In
J.
0.
Cooper,
T.
E.
Heron,
&
W.
L.
Heward
(Eds.),
Applied
behavior
analysis
(pp.
298-326).
Columbus,
OH:
Merrill.
Dunlap,
G.,
&
Plienis,
A.
J.
(1988).
Generalization
and
maintenance
of
unsupervised
responding
via
remote
con-
tingencies.
In
R.
H.
Homer,
G.
Dunlap,
&
R.
L.
Koegel
(Eds.),
Generalization
and
maintenance:
Life-style
changes
in
applied
settings
(pp.
121-142).
Baltimore:
Paul
H.
Brookes.
Fowler,
S.
A.
(1988).
The
effects
of
peer-mediated
inter-
ventions
on
establishing,
maintaining,
and
generalizing
children's
behavior
changes.
In
R.
H.
Horner,
G.
Dunlap,
&
R.
L.
Koegel
(Eds.),
Generalization
and
mainte-
nance:
Life-style
changes
in
applied
settings
(pp.
143-
170).
Baltimore:
Paul
H.
Brookes.
Foxx,
R.
M.,
&
Azrin,
N.
H.
(1973).
Toilet
training
the
retarded:
A
rapid
program
for
day
and
nighttime
in-
dependent
toileting.
Champaign,
IL:
Research
Press.
Frank,
A.
R.,
Wacker,
D.
P.,
Berg,
W.
K.,
&
McMahon,
C.
M.
(1985).
Teaching
selected
microcomputer
skills
to
retarded
students
via
picture
prompts.
Journal
of
Applied
Behavior
Analysis,
18,
179-185.
Johnson,
B.,
&
Cuvo,
A.
(1981).
Teaching
mentally
re-
tarded
adults
to
cook.
Behavior
Modification,
5,
187-
202.
Martin,
J.,
Rusch,
F.,
James,
V.,
Decker,
P.,
&
Trtol,
K.
(1982).
The
use
of
picture
cues
to
establish
self-control
in
the
preparation
of
complex
meals
by
mentally
retarded
adults.
Applied
Research
in
Mental
Retardation,
3,
105-119.
McClannahan,
L.
E.,
Krantz,
P.
J.,
McGee,
G.
G.,
&
MacDuff,
G.
S.
(1984).
Teaching-Family
Model
for autistic
chil-
dren.
In
W.
P.
Christian,
G.
T.
Hannah,
&
T.
J.
Glahn
(Eds.),
Programming
efective
human
services:
Strategies
for
institutional
change
and
client
transition
(pp.
383-
406).
New
York:
Plenum.
Robinson-Wilson,
M.
A.
(1977).
Picture
recipe
cards
as
an
approach
to
teaching
severely
and
profoundly
retarded
adults
to
cook.
In
M.
E.
Snell
(Ed.),
Systematic
instruc-
tion
of
the
moderately
and
severely
handicapped
(pp.
69-73).
Columbus,
OH:
Merrill.
Sailor,
W.,
Goetz,
L.,
Anderson,
J.,
Hunt,
P.,
&
Gee,
K.
(1988).
Research
on
community
intensive
instruction
as
a
model
for
building
functional,
generalized
skills.
In
R.
H.
Homer,
G.
Dunlap,
&
R.
L.
Koegel
(Eds.),
Gen-
eralization
and
maintenance:
Life-style
changes
in
ap-
plied
settings
(pp.
67-98).
Baltimore:
Paul
H.
Brookes.
Snell,
M.
E.
(1983).
Implementing
and
monitoring
the
IEP:
Intervention
strategies.
In
M.
E.
Snell
(Ed.),
Sys-
tematic
instruction
of
the
moderately
and
severely
hand-
icapped
(pp.
113-145).
Columbus,
OH:
Merrill.
Sowers,J.,
Rusch,
F.,
Connis,
R.,
&
Cummings,
L.
(1980).
Teaching
mentally
retarded
adults
to
time-manage
in
a
vocational
setting.
Journal
of
Applied
Behavior
Anal-
ysis,
13,
119-128.
Thinesen,
P.,
&
Bryan,
A.
(1981).
The
use
of
sequential
picture
cues
in
the
initiation
and
maintenance
of
groom-
ing
behaviors
with
mentally
retarded
adults.
Mental
Re-
tardation,
19,
246-250.
Wacker,
D.
P.,
&
Berg,
W.
K.
(1983).
Effects
of
picture
prompts on
the
acquisition
of
complex
vocational
tasks
by
mentally
retarded
adolescents.
Journal
of
Applied
Behavior
Analysis,
16,
417-433.
Wacker,
D.
P.,
Berg,
W.
K.,
Berrie,
P.,
&
Swatta,
P.
(1985).
Generalization
and
maintenance
of
complex
skills
by
se-
verely
handicapped
adolescents
following
picture
prompt
training.
Journal
of
Applied
Behavior
Analysis,
18,
329-336.
Received
July
15,
1991
Initial
editorial
decision
September
23,
1991
Revisions
received
December
23,
1991;
March
3,
1992;
October
20,
1992
Final
acceptance
November
9,
1992
Action
Editor,
F.
Charles
Mace